ARTÍCULOS MÉDICOS

Columna vertebral

Deformations of abdominal muscles under experimentally induced low back pain

European Spine Journal

November 2019, Volume 28, Issue 11, pp 2444–2451|

Biały, M., Adamczyk, W.M., Marczykowski, P. et al. Eur Spine J (2019) 28: 2444.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-019-06016-y

Deformations of abdominal muscles under experimentally induced low back pain

Abstract

Purpose

Low back pain (LBP) is associated with altered motor control and muscle activity; however, it remains unknown whether these changes predispose humans to injury and pain or are the consequence of ongoing nociceptive processes. In this experimental study, we aimed to use a novel ultrasound imaging technique for the measurement of lateral abdominal wall muscle activity: the tissue deformation index (TDI).

Methods

Forty-two healthy subjects (22.30 ± 1.49 years of age) were exposed to postural perturbation induced by rapid arm movement. Activity of three muscles, the transversus abdominis (TrA) and the internal and external oblique (EO), was recorded by ultrasound imaging (M-mode) with and without induction of LBP. Pain was induced by electrical stimulation applied bilaterally to the lumbar spine.

Results

No significant differences in the TDI between right and left sides of the body (p > 0.05) were found. Generally, muscles deformed slower with pain compared to non-painful conditions; however, only EO muscle displayed a statistically significant reduction in deformation velocity (p ≤ 0.00001). TDI for EO decreased from 0.065% per ms ( ± 0.038, 95% CI 0.057–0.074) to 0.053% per ms ( ± 0.035, 95% CI 0.046–0.061). Furthermore, characteristic inter-muscular TDI gradients were observed, directed from inner towards outer muscular layers, with TrA showing the lowest TDI and EO the highest.

Conclusion

Experimentally induced LBP suppresses deformation of the superficial abdominal muscles and increases the variability of local/deep muscles. Further research is required to confirm these observations.

Graphical abstract

These slides can be retrieved under Electronic Supplementary Material.

Keywords

Abdominal muscles Acute low back pain Experimental pain Motor control Ultrasound imaging 

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article ( https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-019-06016-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Introduction

Low back pain (LBP) is associated with altered motor control and muscle activity. A number of studies have shown a link between LBP and delayed muscle onsets [1], lower level of motor variability [2], reduced muscle activation of the trunk, increased stiffness [3], lower muscle force [4], changes in muscles’ voluntary activity [5] and higher-ordered movement capacities, such as gait [67]. Interestingly, although all of these behavioural features have their own underlying physiological changes (e.g. back muscles atrophy, fat infiltration), it is still unknown whether these changes predispose humans to injury and pain or only pose consequences of the ongoing nociceptive processes. One way to disentangle this problem is to fully understand how the motor system responds to acute LBP in an experimental setting. In previous studies on LBP, the role of deep abdominal muscles has been highlighted, especially the role of transversus abdominis (TrA) and inert parts of internal oblique (IO) muscles [8]. Some studies have shown that activation of these muscles is reduced in LBP [89], while other reported contrary results [10]. Nevertheless, there is a consensus related to the difference between patients with back pain and healthy controls, suggesting that people change the way they move during painful events [11]. It has been suggested that contemporary models of movement alterations in pain are not consistently supported by the evidence, which leads to different conclusions in research reports [11]. For example, Lund’s adaptation to pain theory assumes that some motor units are inhibited in pain, i.e. these related to the source of nociception, while those units responsible for innervation of antagonistic muscles reveal enhancement in activity [12]. Although this model has been widely accepted, there are studies which either confirm or disconfirm it (see [11] for review). The second theory, the vicious circle theory, states that pain produces stereotypic responses of increased muscle activation, which leads to stiffness, ischaemia and subsequent painful events [13]. Recently, Hodges and Tucker [11] questioned both theories and argued that both concepts are too simple to explain huge variability in motor responses to nociception at micro and macro level. They argued that new theory should explain motor behaviour change in terms of protection to the body. In this experimental study, we aimed to further investigate motor control mechanisms of lateral abdominal wall muscles (LAMs) in acute LBP by using a novel ultrasound (US) method for the measurement of muscle activity: a tissue deformation index (TDI). This new method measures velocity of muscle deformation during motor tasks and recently has been shown to be reliable and valid [14]. The aim of this study was to investigate TDI changes during LAMs activation under experimentally induced pain. Since abdominal muscles are important from the perspective of lumbar spine function, the ultimate goal of this study was to provide evidence supporting theories regarding the influence of pain on motor control patterns of the above-mentioned musculature.

Materials and methods

Participants

Out of 53 volunteers who answered our recruiting announcement, 48 subjects were qualified (19 women). Participants were excluded if they reported any of the following: (1) history of serious pain and injury within the lumbo-pelvic area and lower limbs (requiring medical assistance of at least 2-week duration; n = 2), (2) history of any surgical intervention, pain or functional limitations within the upper limbs, or exercise training engaging lumbar and abdominal musculature (Pilates, yoga, Tai Chi, Australian approach, etc.) two months prior to the measurement (n = 2), (3) pain or other minor bodily symptoms (e.g. headache, post-exercise fatigue, cold, etc.) on examination day (n = 1). After data collection, image quality was initially screened by a third independent person (a qualified physiotherapist). As a result, images from 6 subjects were disqualified due to movement-induced artefacts in the tissue echogenicity, leading to inaccurate discrimination between fascial/muscle tissue. Finally, data were analysed from 42 subjects (16 women) characterized by a mean age of 22.30 ± 1.49 years, body height of 177.16 ± 7.30 cm, body mass of 71.76 ± 10.66 kg and BMI of 22.75 ± 2.16 (Table 1). The research was approved by the local Bioethical Committee (No. 5/2011), and all subjects signed written informed consent according to Helsinki Declaration before participation in this experiment.Table 1

Demographic characteristics of the enrolled subjects (N = 42)

Variable 
Male/female ratio16:26
Patient age (years)a22.30 ± 1.49
Height (m)a177.16 ± 7.30
Weight (kg)a71.76 ± 10.66
Body mass index (kg/m2)a22.75 ± 2.16

aValues expressed as mean ± standard deviation

Raters

Two raters who were blinded to the objective of the study were involved in the data collection. The raters underwent a 4-week training in ultrasound (US) LAM measurement (3 h, 3 times a week). This training was guided by an experienced specialist (author and principal investigator in the several projects using US measurements) who was not directly involved in the study. While one of the raters was operating the array, the second was responsible for recording the US images (tasks were randomly assigned). Afterwards one of the raters performed the data readout from the recorded US images. In addition, the third person involved in the study supervised the procedure, recruited subjects, screened them against the selection criteria and encoded the US images. (All images were blinded to the rater performing the readout.)

Measurement

A Mindray DP 6600 US imaging device (Mindray, Shenzhen, China) equipped with a linear array (frequency range 5–10 MHz) was used in the measurements. The images were obtained at a frequency of 5 MHz, as was recommended for LAMs measurements [15-17]. In order to localize the optimal site for LAMs, imaging B-mode was engaged. Then, the device was switched to M-mode, and the final image of the LAMs was recorded in JPG format [141718].

Using this method, we evaluated activation of the LAMs in response to postural perturbation in the form of rapid arm abduction with a weight of 3 kg held in the subject’s hand, triggered by an auditory stimulus [19]; this was performed with and without induction of experimental pain. The abduction movement was chosen for two reasons: (1) during the pilot study, the images recorded during arm abduction were less susceptible to graphical distortions, and (2) non-rotation movement acts on the trunk during arm abduction; this movement, present during arm flexion/extension, in large part is parallel to the direction of LAM fibres (especially TrA) and may introduce movement-direction-dependent changes in the activation pattern [20].

The subjects assumed an erect position, with their feet 23 cm apart, sight fixed on a point marked on the wall and arms at their sides. With the US array positioned horizontally [21], one rater identified the optimal location for LAM imaging. Starting from the navel, he gradually moved the array in the lateral direction until the desired image of the three layers of the LAMs appeared on the screen [17]. The optimal array location was marked on the skin with a piece of elastic tape (attached freely, without any strain) with an opening matching the array shape. If necessary, the array’s position could have been reproduced easily. Subsequently, the US device was switched to M-mode. Each subject took one 3 kg weight in one hand and performed three preparatory repetitions of the rapid arm abduction up to 90°, triggered by an auditory stimulus in the earphones worn by the subjects. The use of this type of stimulus was necessary in order to synchronize the movement with the start of US image recording. Also, earphones reduced the risk of detecting other interfering sounds from the environment. If no problem was met and the image quality was acceptable, the ‘true’ recording was initiated. The second rater then gave the command, ‘get ready’ to the subjects. The auditory stimulus occurred every 2–6 s (in a random order) after that. Six repetitions of arm abduction were performed, and six M-mode US images were gathered. This constituted series 1 of the measurements. After a 5-min rest, an identical series (called series 2) with experimentally induced pain was started. For technical reasons, LAMs contralateral to the moving arm were imaged [22]. The side of the body on which the measurements began was randomized. The described methodology is a modified version of the procedures published by other authors [22-25]. The modifications included: stimulus type (auditory vs. Visual), arm movement direction (abduction vs. Flexion) and initial incorporation of B-mode US to localize the optimal site for LAM imaging. The measurements were conducted in a laboratory under standardized environmental conditions. Reliability of the described method was previously reported as good to excellent (intraclass correlation coefficient values for LAMs’ TDI were: TrA: 0.81, IO: 0.88 and EO: 0.87) [14].

Experimentally induced pain

Pain was induced using a Transcutaneous Electrical Nerve Stimulator (frequency 120 Hz, impulse width 15–250 μs) modulated sinusoidally to avoid receptors’ adaptation. Stimulation was delivered through two adhesive electrodes (5 × 10 cm) attached bilaterally to the lower back (above the posterior iliac spines, Fig. 1) where patients with LBP typically experience their pain [26]. To ensure equal intensity of experienced pain, the level of stimulation was individually calibrated prior to postural disturbance according to method of limits [2728]. Thus, the level of stimulation was gradually increased (in mA units) until participants reported moderate pain intensity corresponding to the score of 60% on the Visual Analogue Scale [29]. Participants were thoroughly instructed that stimulation might be manifested by itching, scratching, burning or stinging, but their task was only to evaluate their personal pain perception, not other sensory experiences. They were also informed about the scale anchors, with ‘0’ standing for no pain at all and ‘100’ for the worst pain imaginable. During postural disturbance, pain intensity was set at 60%. The painful stimulation was immediately released after finishing the US data collection. No adverse events were reported.

Fig. 1
Fig. 1Adhesive electrodes placement during the painful stimulation. The lower lateral corner of each electrode overlaid the respective posterior superior iliac spine (PSIS)

Data processing

In total, 24 US images of the LAMs were gathered from each subject (12 each from series 1 and 2). These were screened for image quality by a third person. The two images of lowest quality were excluded from each of body site in series 1 and 2. Thus, a total of sixteen images from each subject (two series of 8 images) were ultimately subjected to analysis. To reduce bias, the names of the qualified images were encoded. The images were transferred to a PC computer and analysed using Photoshop 8.0 software (Adobe, San José, USA). The contrast ( + 75%) and zoom ( × 10) of the images were adjusted. Then, using Photoshop tools, the following measurements were taken for each of the LAMs (TrA, IO, EO): muscle thickness at rest, muscle thickness at the point of maximal activation (both along the y-axis of the image) and time to achieve maximal activation (along the x-axis of the image; Fig. 2). Both image axes were scaled so as to offer accurate information about time and spatial parameters.

Fig. 2
Fig. 2Both images after contrast correction ( + 75%) with Photoshop software. Left image is a real‐time M‐mode sonogram in pain-free condition. EO, external oblique muscle; IO, internal oblique muscle; ST, subcutaneous tissue; TrA, transversus abdominis. Right image presents an M-mode sonogram during the experimentally induced pain condition. As an example, measurements performed on the internal oblique muscle are presented. The thickness of the muscle at rest was measured along the TR (thickness at rest) line between the inner borders of the inter-muscular fascia. The thickness at maximal activation was measured along the TA (thickness at activation) line at the point of the most downward excursion of the fascia, located deep to the given muscle. The time to maximal activation was measured along the T (time) line, between the TR and TA lines. Both image axes were scaled to offer accurate information about time and spatial parameters. The measurement procedure was identical for both pain-free and painful conditions for the external and internal obliques and the transversus abdominis muscles

Based on the three mentioned measurements, the TDI was calculated for each individual LAM for both series according to the following formula:TDI=((TA/TR×100%)−100%)×T−1TDI=((TA/TR×100%)−100%)×T−1where TDI is tissue deformation index (% per ms); TR, muscle thickness at rest (mm); TA, muscle thickness at maximal activation (mm); T, time to achieve maximal activation (ms).

Statistical analysis

Normality of distributions was checked using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Subsequently, differences in TDI between pain-free and painful conditions were compared using a parametric paired Student test. Inter-muscle comparisons of the TDI were performed using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with ‘muscle’ (TrA, IO, EO) set as a between subject factor, followed by the post hoc Tukey test. Alpha level was set at 0.05. For all calculations, Statistica software was applied (Statistica, Tulsa, USA).

Results

Since there were no significant differences of the TDI between right and left sides of the body (Student t test, p > 0.05), results concerning the two sides were pooled and analysed together. Generally, a decrease in the mean TDI was recorded between the pain-free and painful conditions (Fig. 2). For TrA, the TDI decreased from 0.026% ( ± 0.049, 95% CI 0.015–0.037) to 0.023% ( ± 0.045, 95% CI 0.013–0.033) per ms, respectively. For IO, TDI decreased from 0.037% ( ± 0 0.029, 95% CI 0.031–0.043) to 0.033% ( ± 0.024, 95% CI 0.028–0.038) per ms, and for EO it decreased from 0.065% ( ± 0.038, 95% CI 0.057–0.074) to 0.053% ( ± 0.035, 95% CI 0.046–0.061) per ms. Only in the case of the EO was the recorded decrease statistically significant (Student test; t(83) = − 4.72; p = 0.00001).A characteristic inter-muscular gradient of the TDIs was observed, directed form inner towards outer muscular layers, with TrA presenting the lowest TDI (i.e. lowest velocity of thickness increase) and EO the highest (Fig. 3). In both pain-free and painful conditions, the ANOVA showed significant results (F(2, 249) = 30.09; p < 0.00001 and F(2, 249) = 20.74; p < 0.00001, respectively). For the pain-free condition, TDIs characterizing all addressed muscles were significantly different (Tukey test, TrA vs. IO p < 0.05; TrA vs. EO p < 0.0001; IO vs. EO p < 0.0001). The same was recorded for the painful condition (Tukey test, TrA vs. IO p < 0.05; TrA vs. EO p < 0.0001; IO vs. EO p < 0.001; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
Fig. 3Mean values of the tissue deformation index (TDI) for all analysed muscles in the control condition (pain-free) and with pain (painful stimulation). Whiskers indicate standard error of measurement (SEM) values. TrA, transversus abdominis; IO, internal oblique; EO, external oblique. Only in the case of EO was the inter-condition difference statistically significant (***p = 0.00001)

Interestingly, a gradient occurred also in the variability of TDIs, with TrA showing the highest variability (the highest standard deviations, see above), and EO the lowest. For the pain-free condition, 26.19% of the subjects (n = 22) had negative TDI values for TrA, while for the painful condition 29.76% (n = 25). For IO, 5.95% (n = 5) and 1.19% (n = 1) were noted for pain-free and painful conditions, respectively. No negative TDIs were recorded in the case of EO. Additionally, LAMs spatial parameters in pain-free and painful conditions are presented in Table 2.Table 2

Mean values of the lateral abdominal muscles thickness in the control condition (pain-free) and during painful stimulation (painful)

 TrA (mm)IO (mm)EO (mm)
TRTATRTATRTA
Pain-freea4.68 ( ± 1.39)5.16 ( ± 1.80)12.59 ( ± 3.70)14.17 ( ± 3.81)8.54 ( ± 2.18)10.68 ( ± 2.87)
Painfula4.70 ( ± 1.33)5.13 ( ± 1.73)12.96 ( ± 3.57)14.48 ( ± 3.88)8.80 ( ± 2.09)10.61 ( ± 2.62)

TrA transversus abdominis, EO external oblique, IO internal oblique, TR thickness at rest, TA thickness at activation

aValues expressed as mean ± standard deviation

Discussion

The most valuable outcomes of this study were that the obtained TDIs exhibited a characteristic gradient of muscles activation and that the implementation of experimental pain stimulus elicited TDI changes in LAMs. Low TDI values indicate a small change in thickness over time, while high values reflect faster tissue deformation. First, under pain-free conditions, we report a characteristic gradient of increasing TDI values from deep tissue layers (for TrA, 0.026% per ms) towards superficial layers (for IO, 0.037% and for EO, 0.065% per ms), which has also been reported in our previous work [14]. This finding can be explained by physiological adaptations. Superficial muscles are mainly built of fast-twitch muscle fibres [3031], which is reflected in high TDI values (e.g. EO). The deepest muscle (TrA) has a larger share of slow-twitch fibres and presents the lowest deformation index. This observation can also be explained from a functional point of view. Superficial muscles are often regarded as torque and motion producing, while deep muscles are considered stabilizing. Interestingly, this characteristic gradient was observed in pain-free as well as in painful condition, and all recorded TDIs were significantly different. We also noticed a specific gradient in the variability of TDI values in both pain-free and painful conditions independently, with TrA producing the highest score of negative TDI values from 26.19% (pain-free) to 29.76% (painful condition) and IO values from 5.95% (pain-free) to 1.19% (painful condition), with no negative EO TDIs. Transversus abdominis was the muscle that exhibited a decrease in thickness (possibly deactivation) most frequently. These findings are supported by the results of other experiments. Many authors noted that the mechanism of motor control of the deep abdominal muscles is based on random muscle activation in both normal and painful conditions [32-34]. This might suggest that ‘plasticity’ constitutes a characteristic feature of the pattern of deep abdominal muscle activation, and it often depends on the character of the given motor task (e.g. symmetrical or asymmetrical, internal or external forces) or on the environment (e.g. painful condition). Furthermore, none of the investigated muscles showed higher TDI with the pain stimulus. In experimental LBP conditions, we noticed a decrease in TDI mean values (TrA from 0.026 to 0.023% per ms, IO from 0.037 to 0.033% per ms and EO from 0.065 to 0.053% per ms). Only in the case of the EO was the recorded decrease statistically significant (Student test p = 0.00001). These observations might indicate that an implementation of nociceptive input in the lower back region does not interrupt the physiological component of abdominal muscle activation, but influences their motor control, which is pronounced by their deactivation and is consistent with the data available in literature. Several studies have demonstrated that abdominal muscle function is disorganized in special conditions such as LBP [835]. Hodges and Richardson [35] identified delayed activity of TrA with movements of the arm and leg during lower back experimental pain stimulation. Similar results were reported by Ferreira et al. [36], who used change in US measurements of thickness in abdominal muscles to find that people with LBP history use a different motor strategy of trunk control (especially TrA) during isometric lower leg tasks, compared to people without LBP. Our results indicate that there is a trend to decrease TDI values between the pain-free and pain conditions across LAMs, with significant differences in the EO muscle. In contrast to our results, several studies reported that in LBP patients, superficial muscles react with greater activity and decreased fatigability [37], or underline individual variation in EO activity [38]. Meanwhile, in our study we noticed a greater decrease in EO activity, compared to other abdominal muscles, which leads to the thesis that implementation of pain stimulus in lower back area affects motor control of superficial rather than deep muscles during postural perturbations. This finding may be useful in clinical practice and may partially explain the positive influence of general abdominal muscle strengthening exercises on pain and function in LBP patients [3940]. Limitations of our study include the participants. While the group was made up of healthy young subjects, they seemed to serve as a good reference for the symptomatic population. While we did not control the ultrasound array pressure during rapid arm movement, our raters were carefully trained and the recording procedure was fully standardized. Despite these factors, we were able to demonstrate the good reliability of the TDI [14]. The limitation might be the pain model itself. Transcutaneous electrical stimuli were used to induce rather superficial pain sensation. Thus, the source of nociception is limited and cannot be directly compared with true LBP arising from other structural causes, e.g. nerve root compression. It is recommended to replicate our results in clinical populations and healthy subjects exposed to different pain models.

Conclusion

TDI values for lateral abdominal muscles formed a gradient that increased from deep to superficial muscles. This gradient was observed in pain-free and painful conditions. Experimentally induced pain in the lower back influences motor control of the LAMs, which is reflected in the decrease of TDI values in the case of EO. TrA presented the largest percentage of negative TDI values in both conditions, and this is why it can be presumed as the most variable motor ‘unit’ in that muscle group. Further research is needed to investigate muscle deformations in patients with LBP.

Notes

Acknowledgements

WMA is supported by the scholarship ETIUDA5 funded by the National Science Centre in Poland (Grant No. 2017/24/T/HS6/00329).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary material

586_2019_6016_MOESM1_ESM.pptx (607 kb)Supplementary file1 (PPTX 607 kb)

References

  1. 1.Suehiro T, Mizutani M, Ishida H, Kobara K, Osaka H, Watanabe S (2015) Individuals with chronic low back pain demonstrate delayed onset of the back muscle activity during prone hip extension. J Electromyo Kinesiol 25:675–680.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2015.04.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.Abboud J, Nougarou F, Pagé I, Cantin V, Massicotte D, Descarreaux M (2014) Trunk motor variability in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain. Eur J Appl Physiol 114:2645–2654.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-014-2985-8CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.Hodges PW, Van den Hoorn W, Dawson A, Cholewicki J (2009) Changes in the mechanical properties of the trunk in low back pain may be associated with recurrence. J Biomech 42:61–66.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2008.10.001CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.Saur P, Koch D, Steinmetz U, Straub A, Ensink FB, Kettler D, Hildebrandt J (1997) Isokinetic strength of lumbar muscles in patients with chronic backache. Z Orthop Ihre Grenzgeb 13:315–322.  https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1039395CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.Lima M, Ferreira AS, Reis FJJ, Paes V, Meziat-Filho N (2018) Chronic low back pain and back muscle activity during functional tasks. Gait Posture 61:250–256.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.01.021CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.Ghamkhar L, Kahlaee AH (2015) Trunk muscles activation pattern during walking in subjects with and without chronic low back pain: a systematic review. PM&R 7:519–526.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmrj.2015.01.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.Hicks GE, Sions JM, Coyle PC, Pohlig RT (2017) Altered spatiotemporal characteristics of gait in older adults with chronic low back pain. Gait Posture 55:172–176.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.04.027CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.Hodges PW, Richardson CA (1998) Delayed postural contraction of transverses abdominis associated with lower back pain. J Spinal Disord 11:46–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.Moseley GL, Hodges PW (2005) Are the changes in postural control associated with low back pain caused by pain interference? Clin J Pain 21:323–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.Gubler D, Mannion AF, Schenk P, Gorelick M, Helbling D, Gerber H et al (2010) Ultrasound tissue Doppler imaging reveals no delay in abdominal muscle feed-forward activity during rapid arm movements in patients with chronic low back pain. Spine J 35:1506–1513.  https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181c3ed41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.Hodges PW, Tucker K (2011) Moving differently in pain: a new theory to explain the adaptation to pain. Pain 152:S90–S98.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.020CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.Lund JP, Donga R, Widmer CG, Stohler CS (1991) The pain-adaptation model: a discussion of the relationship between chronic musculoskeletal pain and motor activity. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 69:683–694CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.Roland MO (1986) A critical review of the evidence for a pain-spasm-pain cycle in spinal disorders. Clin Biomech 1:102–109.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0268-0033(86)90085-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.Biały M, Adamczyk WM, Gnat R, Stranc T (2017) Tissue deformation index as a reliable measure of lateral abdominal muscle activation on M-mode sonography. J Ultrasound Med 36:1461–1467.  https://doi.org/10.7863/ultra.16.07045CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.Teyhen DS, Miltenberger CE, Deiters HM, Del Toro YM, Pulliam JN, Childs JD et al (2005) The use of ultrasound imaging of the abdominal drawing-in maneuver in subjects with low back pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 35:346–355.  https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2005.35.6.346CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.Ferreira PH, Ferreira ML, Nascimento DP, Pinto RZ, Franco MR (2009) Discriminative and reliability analyses of ultrasound measurement of abdominal muscle recruitment. Man Ther 16:463–469.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2011.02.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.Gnat R, Saulicz E, Miądowicz B (2012) Reliability of real-time ultrasound measurement of transversus abdominis thickness in healthy trained subjects. Eur Spine J 21:1508–1515.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-012-2184-4CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.Adamczyk WM, Biały M, Stranc T (2016) Asymmetry of lateral abdominal wall muscles in static and dynamic conditions: a preliminary study of professional basketball players. Sci Sports 31:e15–e18.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scispo.2015.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.Crommert ME, Ekblom MM, Thorstensson A (2011) Activation of transversus abdominis varies with postural demand in standing. Gait Posture 33:473–477.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.12.028CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.Allison GT, Morris SL, Lay B (2008) Feedforward responses of transversus abdominis are directionally specific and act asymmetrically: implications for core stability theories. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 38:228–237.  https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2008.2703CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.Urquhart DM, Barker PJ, Hodges PW, Story IH, Briggs CA (2005) Regional morphology of the transversus abdominis and obliquus internus and externus abdominis muscles. Clin Biomech 20:233–241.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2004.11.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.Vasseljen O, Fladmark AM, Westad C, Torp HG (2009) Onset in abdominal muscles recorded simultaneously by ultrasound imaging and intramuscular electromyography. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 19:23–31.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2007.07.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.Bunce SM, Moore AP, Hough AD (2002) M-mode ultrasound: a reliable measure of transversus abdominis thickness? Clin Biomech 17:315–317.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0268-0033(02)00011-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.McMeeken JM, Beith ID, Newham DJ, Milligan P, Critchley DJ (2004) The relationship between EMG and change in thickness of transversus abdominis. Clin Biomech 19:337–342.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2004.01.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.Westad C, Mork PJ, Vasseljen O (2009) Location and sequence of muscle onset in deep abdominal muscles measured by different modes of ultrasound imaging. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 20:994–999.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2010.01.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.Thiese MS, Hegmann KT, Wood EM, Garg A, Moore JS, Kapel
    lusch J et al (2014) Prevalence of low back pain by anatomic location and intensity in an occupational population. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 21(15):283.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2474-15-283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.Gracely RH, Lota L, Walter DJ, Dubner R (1988) A multiple random staircase method of psychophysical pain assessment. Pain 32:55–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.Bąbel P, Adamczyk WM, Swider K, Bajcar EA, Kicman P, Lisinska N (2017) How classical conditioning shapes placebo analgesia: hidden versus open conditioning. Pain Med 19:1156–1169.  https://doi.org/10.1093/pm/pnx177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.Boonstra AM, Schiphorst Preuper HR, Balk GA, Stewart RE (2014) Cut-off points for mild, moderate, and severe pain on the visual analogue scale for pain in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain. Pain 155:2545–2550.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.09.014CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.Haggmark T, Thorstensson A (1979) Fibre types in human abdominal muscles. Acta Physiol Scand 107:319–325.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-1716.1979.tb06482.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.Verbout AJ, Wintzen AR, Linthont P (1989) The distribution of slow and fast twitch fibres in the intrinsic lumbar back muscles. Clin Anat 2:120–121Google Scholar
  32. 32.Lehman GJ, Story S, Mabee R (2005) Influence of static lumbar flexion on the trunk muscles response to sudden arm movements. Chiropr Osteopat 23:13–23.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1746-1340-13-23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.Marshall P, Murphy B (2006) Effects of sacroiliac joint manipulation on feedforward times of deep abdominal musculature. J Manip Physiol Ther 29:96–202.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmpt.2006.01.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.Jacobs JV, Henry SM, Nagle KJ (2009) People with chronic low back pain exhibit decreased variability in the timing of their anticipatory postural adjustments. Behav Neurosci 123:455–458.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014479CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.Hodges PW, Richardson CA (1996) Inefficient muscular stabilization of the lumbar spine associated with low back pain: a motor control evaluation of transverses abdominis. Spine 21:2640–2650CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.Ferreira PH, Ferreira ML, Hodges PW (2004) Changes in recruitment of the abdominal muscles in people with low back pain: ultrasound measurement of muscle activity. Spine 15:2560–2566CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.Ng JK, Richardson CA, Parnianpour M, Kippers V (2002) EMG activity of trunk muscles and torque output during isometric axial rotation exertion: a comparison between back pain patients and matched controls. J Orthop Res 20:112–121.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0736-0266(01)00067-5CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.Hodges PW, Pengel LH, Herbert RD, Gandevia SC (2003) Measurement of muscle contraction with ultrasound imaging. Muscle Nerve 27:682–692CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.Moon HJ, Choi KH, Kim DH, Kim HJ, Cho YK, Lee KH et al (2013) Effect of lumbar stabilization and dynamic lumbar strengthening exercises in patients with chronic low back pain. Ann Rehabil Med 37:110–117.  https://doi.org/10.5535/arm.2013.37.1.110CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. 40.Knox MF, Chipchase LS, Schabrun SM, Marshall PWM (2017) Improved compensatory postural adjustments of the deep abdominals following exercise in people with chronic low back pain. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 37:117–124.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2017.10.009CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2019

Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

About this article

CrossMarkCite this article as:Biały, M., Adamczyk, W.M., Marczykowski, P. et al. Eur Spine J (2019) 28: 2444. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-019-06016-y

  • Received20 February 2019
  • Revised02 April 2019
  • Accepted16 May 2019
  • First Online24 May 2019
  • DOIhttps://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-019-06016-y
  • Publisher NameSpringer Berlin Heidelberg
  • Print ISSN0940-6719
  • Online ISSN1432-0932

Introduce tu búsqueda en el cajetín para encontrar contenido.

Ir al contenido